25298
Examining Cognitive Inflexibility and Anxiety in Relation to Restricted, Repetitive Behaviours in Autism Spectrum Disorders in a Large Multi-Site Study

Friday, May 12, 2017: 1:45 PM
Yerba Buena 7 (Marriott Marquis Hotel)
D. V. Crawley1, J. Ahmad2, H. den Ouden3, G. Dumas4, J. Tillmann5, A. San Jose Caceres6, T. Charman7, J. K. Buitelaar3, D. G. Murphy8 and E. Loth9, (1)Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, King's College, London, London, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland, (2)Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, United Kingdom, (3)Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Centre, Nijmegen, Netherlands, (4)Institut Pasteur, Paris, France, (5)King's College London, London, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland, (6)Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, King's College, London, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland, (7)Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom, (8)Department of Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, and the Sackler Institute for Translational Neurodevelopment, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom, (9)Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
Background:

Restricted and repetitive behaviours (RRBs) are core features of autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Previous studies have suggested distinct subtypes of RRBs, such as ‘lower-order’ (e.g. motor stereotypies) and ‘higher-order’ RRBs (e.g. restricted interests and insistence on sameness; Turner, 1999; Rutter et al., 2003). Despite various descriptions of RRBs, their origins are less well understood. This may be because: (1) different facets of RRBs have different underpinnings; or (2) the same facet has different underpinnings in different individuals. As an example: for some, cognitive inflexibility may underlie insistence on sameness (D’Cruz et al., 2013), whereas for others, anxiety in uncertain situations may be a factor (Lidstone et al., 2014).

Objectives:

(1) To examine the profile and severity of RRBs in children, adolescents and adults who participated in the EU-AIMS Longitudinal European Autism Project (LEAP); and (2) to investigate cognitive inflexibility and anxiety as possible underpinnings of RRBs.

Methods:

Participants were 431 individuals with ASD aged 6-30 years and 298 age-matched controls. RRBs were measured using the Repetitive Behaviour Scale-Revised (RBS-R). The RBS-R restricted interests and rituals/sameness subscales formed the ‘higher-order’ RRBs composite and the stereotyped and self-injurious behaviour subscales formed the ‘lower-order’ composite. IQ was measured using the Wechsler Scales and adaptive behaviour using the Vineland Scales. Cognitive inflexibility was assessed as the number of perseverative errors made on a computer-based probabilistic reversal learning (PRL) task (den Ouden et al., 2013). We classified perseverative errors made by ASD individuals using a normative modelling approach, which assessed each ASD participant’s performance as the deviation from the age-estimated control group mean. Anxiety was measured using the Beck’s Anxiety Inventory.

Results:

As expected, ASD participants scored significantly higher than controls on the RBS-R (p<.001, d=2.06). Within the ASD group: (1) participants had significantly greater ‘higher-order’ RRBs than ‘lower-order’ RRBs (p<.001, d=0.86; Figure 1); (2) RBS-R total scores significantly decreased with age (r=-.25, p<.001); and (3) RBS-R total scores were significantly negatively related to adaptive behaviour, independent of IQ (r=-.31, p<.001). On the PRL task, the ASD group made significantly more perseverative errors than the control group (p<.001, d=0.35). However, when stratifying ASD individuals (Figure 2), 62% performed within +/-1SD of the control group mean, and 91% within +/-2SDs. Only 9% performed below 2SDs. No significant relationship was found between perseverative errors and RRBs in the ASD group, nor did the 9% with extremely high perseveration differ in their RRBs profiles. Across age groups, RRBs – and in children notably rituals/sameness behaviours – were significantly related to levels of anxiety in ASD individuals without intellectual disability (all r’s>.41,p’s<.001).

Conclusions:

We found no evidence for a role of perseveration in RRBs in ASD. Despite significant case-control differences, only a small ‘subgroup’ of ASD participants demonstrated clinically meaningful (<2SDs) impairments in cognitive inflexibility; and they were unrelated to RRBs. Instead our findings provide proof of concept that anxiety may be a main driver of RRBs, and particularly insistence on sameness. If correct, this suggests an alternative treatment approach in ASD – targeting anxiety to reduce RBBs.