26347
Identifying Cultural Differences and Commonalities in Autistic Traits across India, Japan and the UK Using the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ)

Oral Presentation
Saturday, May 12, 2018: 3:16 PM
Arcadis Zaal (de Doelen ICC Rotterdam)
S. Carruthers1, E. Kinnaird2, A. Rudra3, P. Smith4, C. Allison5, B. Auyeung6, B. Chakrabarti7, A. Wakabayashi8, S. Baron-Cohen5, I. Bakolis2 and R. A. Hoekstra9, (1)King's College London, London, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland, (2)King's College London, London, United Kingdom, (3)Psychology, Ben Gurion University of the Negev, Beer Sheva, Israel, (4)University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom, (5)Autism Research Centre, Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom, (6)University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom, (7)Centre for Autism, School of Psychology & Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Reading, United Kingdom, (8)Chiba University, Chiba, Japan, (9)Department of Psychology, King's College London, London, United Kingdom
Background: There is a global need for brief open-source screening instruments that can identify “red flags” for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and support frontline professionals in their referral decision-making (Durkin et al., 2015). Though generally believed to be a universal disorder, there may be subtle differences in identification or reporting of ASD symptoms across cultures. In order to assess the potential for any measure to be adapted for cross-cultural screening use, it is important to understand the relative performance of such measures in different cultures.

Objectives: Our study aimed to identify and compare the items on the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ)-Child (Auyeung et al., 2008) most predictive of an ASD diagnosis among children aged 4-9 years across different samples from India, Japan and the UK.

Methods: Parent-reported AQ-Child data were collected from 73 children with a formal ASD diagnosis and 81 neurotypical children from India (previously reported in Rudra et al., 2014); 116 ASD and 190 neurotypical children from Japan (data not previously reported), and 488 ASD and 532 neurotypical children from the UK (some data previously reported in Allison et al., 2012). None of the children included had a reported diagnosis of intellectual disability. Participants from each country were randomly allocated to derivation and validation samples. For each item, Discrimination Indices (DI) and Positive Predictive Values (PPV) were calculated using the derivation and validation samples respectively. Items surpassing a discrimination index (DI) threshold of 0.5 and a positive predictive value (PPV) of 0.7 within each country were considered highly discriminative for that culture. Such items were compared across cultures. The psychometric validity of the “red flag items” was assessed using Receiving Operating Curves (ROC curves), discriminant analysis, Cronbach’s alpha and Pearson’s correlation coefficient.

Results: 16 items in the Indian sample, 15 items in the Japanese sample and 28 items in the UK sample demonstrated excellent predictive ability of an ASD diagnosis. Five items surpassed the high discrimination threshold (DI≥0.5, PPV≥0.7) in all three samples. One item (‘When s/he talks, it isn’t always easy for others to get a word in edgeways’) was highly discriminative in Japan but poorly discriminative (DI<0.3) in the UK and India, and a further item (S/he enjoys doing things spontaneously) had excellent discrimination properties in the UK but poorly discriminated in the Indian and Japanese samples. Two additional items were highly discriminative in two cultures but poor in the third.

Conclusions: In a cross-cultural study of children with intelligence in the normal range, there was cross-cultural overlap in the items most predictive of an autism diagnosis, supporting the notion of global universality in autistic traits and the possibility of adapting an existing tool for cross-cultural screening. Subtle cultural differences were suggested in four items, which may be related to contrasting sociocultural values and attitudes such as social conformity and uncertainty avoidance (Hofstede, 2001). These findings can inform the development of a brief “red flag” global screening tool for ASD.