29868
Exploring New Outcome Measures to Assess Effectiveness of the PEERS® Social Skills Intervention

Poster Presentation
Friday, May 3, 2019: 5:30 PM-7:00 PM
Room: 710 (Palais des congres de Montreal)
N. L. Matthews1, C. Mortensen1, S. N. Rife1, B. Orr1, K. Warriner2, M. DeCarlo1 and C. J. Smith3, (1)Southwest Autism Research and Resource Center, Phoenix, AZ, (2)Comprehensive Behavioral Programs, Southwest Autism Research and Resource Center, Phoenix, AZ, (3)Southwest Autism Research & Resource Center, Phoenix, AZ
Background: Most studies that indicate effectiveness of PEERS®, a manualized social skills intervention for adolescents with autism (ASD; Laugeson & Frankel, 2010), rely on parent-, adolescent-, and/or teacher-report questionnaires to document outcomes. This may introduce bias because participants are not blind to their study group.

Objectives: To utilize structured conversation probes coded by raters blind to study group and time point to compare change in (a) adherence to the rules for two-way conversations taught during PEERS, and (b) social likeability among participants who were randomly assigned to complete PEERS, a peer-mediated model of PEERS (PMP), or a delayed treatment control (DTC) group.

Methods: Participants were 34 adolescents with ASD (age M = 15.23, SD = 1.18; 82% male) and 16 typically developing (TD) peer mentors (age M = 15.50, SD = 1.27; 44% male). Participants with ASD met criteria for autism/autism spectrum on the ADOS-2 and had an overall or verbal IQ ≥ 70. PEERS was delivered as manualized to the PEERS group. It was delivered identically to participants in PMP, with the exception of the inclusion of TD peer mentors. DTC was assessed at 3 time points over 9 months.

Participants (ASD and TD) completed a videotaped 10-minute structured conversation probe with a novel research assistant pre- and post-intervention. Participants with ASD completed additional probes at 4-month and 1-year follow-ups. It was not possible to collect 1-year probes from DTC participants because they had already started treatment.

Adherence to 16 rules for two-way conversations was coded by trained raters using whole and partial interval recording. Higher scores indicated greater adherence (second rater coded 43%: ICC = .92). To assess social likeability, four novice research assistants watched each video and responded to the statement “What is the likelihood that similar-aged teenagers would enjoy socializing with the participant” using a Likert scale ranging from 1-unlikely to 10-likely. Scores were averaged across raters (ICC = .88).

Results: There was a significant group by time interaction for adherence (Figure 1; F(4, 62) = 2.95, p = .03) and the group by time interaction for social likeability approached significance (Figure 2; F(4, 62) = 2.42, p = .06). Bonferroni post-hoc comparisons indicated that PMP demonstrated greater adherence at immediate and 4-month follow-ups (Times 2 and 3) compared to pre-intervention (ps = .01) and had higher social likeability scores at 4-month follow-up compared to pre-intervention (p = .02). Results from 1-year follow-up (Time 4) data indicated a significant increase from pre-intervention in adherence (p = .01) and social likeability (p < .001) in the PMP group. Adherence and social likeability in PEERS and DTC groups did not differ across time points.

Conclusions: Findings suggest that adolescents in PMP increased adherence to rules for two-way conversations, and that treatment gains were maintained at 4-month and 1-year follow-ups. Treatment gains in adherence were partially corroborated by gains in social likeability. Findings are consistent with previously reported results from adolescent- and parent-report questionnaires from the same sample that indicated a modest advantage for PMP relative to PEERS (blinded).